These are the sources and citations used to research Dissertation about representational pseudoneglect. This bibliography was generated on Cite This For Me on
In-text: (Atkinson and Shiffrin, 1971)
Your Bibliography: Atkinson, R. and Shiffrin, R., 1971. The Control of Short-Term Memory. Sci Am, 225(2), pp.82-90.
In-text: (Baddeley and Hitch, 1994)
Your Bibliography: Baddeley, A. and Hitch, G., 1994. Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), pp.485-493.
In-text: (Baddeley, Lewis and Vallar, 1984)
Your Bibliography: Baddeley, A., Lewis, V. and Vallar, G., 1984. Exploring the articulatory loop. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 36(2), pp.233-252.
In-text: (Beschin, Cocchini, Della Sala and Logie, 1997)
Your Bibliography: Beschin, N., Cocchini, G., Della Sala, S. and Logie, R., 1997. What the Eyes Perceive, The Brain Ignores: A Case of Pure Unilateral Representational Neglect. Cortex, 33(1), pp.3-26.
In-text: (Bisiach and Luzzatti, 1978)
Your Bibliography: Bisiach, E. and Luzzatti, C., 1978. Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space. Cortex, 14(1), pp.129-133.
In-text: (Bowers and Heilman, 1980)
Your Bibliography: Bowers, D. and Heilman, K., 1980. Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 18(4-5), pp.491-498.
In-text: (Bradshaw et al., 1987)
Your Bibliography: Bradshaw, J., Nathan, G., Nettleton, N., Wilson, L. and Pierson, J., 1987. Why is there a left side underestimation in rod bisection?. Neuropsychologia, 25(4), pp.735-738.
In-text: (Brodie and Pettigrew, 1996)
Your Bibliography: Brodie, E. and Pettigrew, L., 1996. Is left always right? Directional deviations in visual line bisection as a function of hand and initial scanning direction. Neuropsychologia, 34(5), pp.467-470.
In-text: (Brodie and Pettigrew, 1996)
Your Bibliography: Brodie, E. and Pettigrew, L., 1996. Is left always right? Directional deviations in visual line bisection as a function of hand and initial scanning direction. Neuropsychologia, 34(5), pp.467-470.
In-text: (Brodie, 2010)
Your Bibliography: Brodie, E., 2010. Reflecting upon line bisection: Mirror reversal increases the magnitude of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 48(5), pp.1517-1520.
In-text: (Brooks and Brandimonte, 2013)
Your Bibliography: Brooks, J. and Brandimonte, M., 2013. Left-ear-driven representational pseudoneglect for mentally represented real-word scenes created from aural–verbal description. Cogn Process, 15(2), pp.201-207.
In-text: (Brooks, 2012)
Your Bibliography: Brooks, J., 2012. Representational pseudoneglect: Lateralised biases in attentional orienting in the absence of vision in healthy ageing participants.. Ph.D. The University of Edinburgh.
In-text: (Brooks, Logie, McIntosh and Sala, 2011)
Your Bibliography: Brooks, J., Logie, R., McIntosh, R. and Sala, S., 2011. Representational pseudoneglect in an auditory-driven spatial working memory task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(11), pp.2168-2180.
In-text: (Bultitude and Aimola Davies, 2006)
Your Bibliography: Bultitude, J. and Aimola Davies, A., 2006. Putting attention on the line: Investigating the activation–orientation hypothesis of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), pp.1849-1858.
In-text: (Cabeza, 2002)
Your Bibliography: Cabeza, R., 2002. Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), pp.85-100.
In-text: (Cattaneo et al., 2011)
Your Bibliography: Cattaneo, Z., Fantino, M., Silvanto, J., Tinti, C. and Vecchi, T., 2011. Blind individuals show pseudoneglect in bisecting numerical intervals. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(4), pp.1021-1028.
In-text: (Chokron and Imbert, 1993)
Your Bibliography: Chokron, S. and Imbert, M., 1993. Egocentric reference and asymmetric perception of space. Neuropsychologia, 31(3), pp.267-275.
In-text: (Danckert and Ferber, 2006)
Your Bibliography: Danckert, J. and Ferber, S., 2006. Revisiting unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia, 44(6), pp.987-1006.
In-text: (Darling, Logie and Della Sala, 2012)
Your Bibliography: Darling, S., Logie, R. and Della Sala, S., 2012. Representational pseudoneglect in line bisection. Psychon Bull Rev, 19(5), pp.879-883.
In-text: (Dehaene, 2003)
Your Bibliography: Dehaene, S., 2003. The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), pp.145-147.
In-text: (Dehaene, Bossini and Giraux, 1993)
Your Bibliography: Dehaene, S., Bossini, S. and Giraux, P., 1993. The Mental Representation of Parity and Number Magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, [online] 122(3), pp.371-396. Available at: <http://www.dc.uba.ar/materias/incc/practicas/p1/Dehaene-ParitySNARCeffect-JEPGeneral1993.pdf> [Accessed 20 March 2015].
In-text: (Della Sala, Darling and Logie, 2010)
Your Bibliography: Della Sala, S., Darling, S. and Logie, R., 2010. Items on the left are better remembered. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(5), pp.848-855.
In-text: (Della Sala et al., 1999)
Your Bibliography: Della Sala, S., Gray, C., Baddeley, A., Allamano, N. and Wilson, L., 1999. Pattern span: a tool for unwelding visuo–spatial memory. Neuropsychologia, 37(10), pp.1189-1199.
In-text: (Della Sala, Logie, Beschin and Denis, 2004)
Your Bibliography: Della Sala, S., Logie, R., Beschin, N. and Denis, M., 2004. Preserved visuo-spatial transformations in representational neglect. Neuropsychologia, 42(10), pp.1358-1364.
In-text: (Dellatolas, Vanluchene and Coutin, 1996)
Your Bibliography: Dellatolas, G., Vanluchene, J. and Coutin, T., 1996. Visual and motor components in simple line bisection: an investigation in normal adults. Cognitive Brain Research, 4(1), pp.49-56.
In-text: (Denis, Goncalves and Memmi, 1995)
Your Bibliography: Denis, M., Goncalves, M. and Memmi, D., 1995. Mental scanning of visual images generated from verbal descriptions: Towards a model of image accuracy. The Neuropsychology of Mental Imagery, 33(11), pp.1511-1530.
In-text: (Dickinson and Intraub, 2009)
Your Bibliography: Dickinson, C. and Intraub, H., 2009. Spatial asymmetries in viewing and remembering scenes: Consequences of an attentional bias?. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(6), pp.1251-1262.
In-text: (GALTON, 1880)
Your Bibliography: GALTON, F., 1880. Visualised Numerals. Nature, 21(536), pp.323-323.
In-text: (Gamberini, Seraglia and Priftis, 2008)
Your Bibliography: Gamberini, L., Seraglia, B. and Priftis, K., 2008. Processing of peripersonal and extrapersonal space using tools: Evidence from visual line bisection in real and virtual environments. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), pp.1298-1304.
In-text: (Guariglia, Padovani, Pantano and Pizzamiglio, 1993)
Your Bibliography: Guariglia, C., Padovani, A., Pantano, P. and Pizzamiglio, L., 1993. Unilateral neglect restricted to visual imagery. Nature, 364(6434), pp.235-237.
In-text: (Hatta and Yamamoyto, 1986)
Your Bibliography: Hatta, T. and Yamamoyto, M., 1986. Hemispheric asymmetries in a tactile bisection task: Effects of hemispace of presentation. Neuropsychologia, 24(2), pp.265-269.
In-text: (Hausmann, Waldie and Corballis, 2003)
Your Bibliography: Hausmann, M., Waldie, K. and Corballis, M., 2003. Developmental changes in line bisection: A result of callosal maturation?. Neuropsychology, 17(1), pp.155-160.
In-text: (Heber et al., 2010)
Your Bibliography: Heber, I., Siebertz, S., Wolter, M., Kuhlen, T. and Fimm, B., 2010. Horizontal and vertical pseudoneglect in peri- and extrapersonal space. Brain and Cognition, 73(3), pp.160-166.
In-text: (Hubbard, Piazza, Pinel and Dehaene, 2005)
Your Bibliography: Hubbard, E., Piazza, M., Pinel, P. and Dehaene, S., 2005. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(6), pp.435-448.
In-text: (Jewell and McCourt, 2000)
Your Bibliography: Jewell, G. and McCourt, M., 2000. Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), pp.93-110.
In-text: (Loftus et al., 2009)
Your Bibliography: Loftus, A., Nicholls, M., Mattingley, J., Chapman, H. and Bradshaw, J., 2009. Pseudoneglect for the bisection of mental number lines. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(5), pp.925-945.
In-text: (Longo and Lourenco, 2006)
Your Bibliography: Longo, M. and Lourenco, S., 2006. On the nature of near space: Effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia, 44(6), pp.977-981.
In-text: (Longo and Lourenco, 2007)
Your Bibliography: Longo, M. and Lourenco, S., 2007. Spatial attention and the mental number line: Evidence for characteristic biases and compression. Neuropsychologia, 45(7), pp.1400-1407.
In-text: (MacLeod and Turnbull, 1997)
Your Bibliography: MacLeod, M. and Turnbull, O., 1997. Motor and perceptual factors in pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, [online] 37, pp.707-713. Available at: <http://turnbull-lab.bangor.ac.uk/turnbull/Oliver%20Turnbull/anosognosia/Pseudoneglect.pdf> [Accessed 20 February 2015].
In-text: (McCourt and Garlinghouse, 2000)
Your Bibliography: McCourt, M. and Garlinghouse, M., 2000. Asymmetries of Visuospatial Attention are Modulated by Viewing Distance and Visual Field Elevation: Pseudoneglect in Peripersonal and Extrapersonal Space. Cortex, 36(5), pp.715-731.
In-text: (McGeorge et al., 2007)
Your Bibliography: McGeorge, P., Beschin, N., Colnaghi, A., Rusconi, M. and Della Sala, S., 2007. A lateralized bias in mental imagery: Evidence for representational pseudoneglect. Neuroscience Letters, 421(3), pp.259-263.
In-text: (Miller, 1956)
Your Bibliography: Miller, G., 1956. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), pp.81-97.
In-text: (NALCACI, CICEK, KALAYCIOGLU and YAVUZER, 1997)
Your Bibliography: NALCACI, E., CICEK, M., KALAYCIOGLU, C. and YAVUZER, S., 1997. PSEUDONEGLECT OF MALES AND FEMALES ON A SPATIAL SHORT-TERM MEMORY TASK. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84(1), pp.99-105.
In-text: (Neath, 2000)
Your Bibliography: Neath, I., 2000. Modeling the effects of irrelevant speech on memory. Psychon Bull Rev, 7(3), pp.403-423.
In-text: (Nicholls and McIlroy, 2009)
Your Bibliography: Nicholls, M. and McIlroy, A., 2009. Spatial Cues Affect Mental Number Line Bisections. Experimental Psychology (formerly Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie), 57(4), pp.315-319.
In-text: (Oliveri et al., 2004)
Your Bibliography: Oliveri, M., Rausei, V., Koch, G., Torriero, S., Turriziani, P. and Caltagirone, C., 2004. Overestimation of numerical distances in the left side of space. Neurology, 63(11), pp.2139-2141.
In-text: (Parra, Cubelli and Della Sala, 2011)
Your Bibliography: Parra, M., Cubelli, R. and Della Sala, S., 2011. Lack of color integration in visual short-term memory binding. Memory & Cognition, 39(7), pp.1187-1197.
In-text: (Parra, Della Sala, Logie and Morcom, 2014)
Your Bibliography: Parra, M., Della Sala, S., Logie, R. and Morcom, A., 2014. Neural correlates of shape–color binding in visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 52, pp.27-36.
In-text: (Peterson and Peterson, 1959)
Your Bibliography: Peterson, L. and Peterson, M., 1959. Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(3), pp.193-198.
In-text: (Philip and Hatwell, 1998)
Your Bibliography: Philip, J. and Hatwell, Y., 1998. Effects of cueing and of the direction of scanning on the tactile line bisection of normal adults. Cahiers De Psychologie Cognitive-Current Psychology of Cognition, 17(1), pp.31-51.
In-text: (Reuter-Lorenz, Kinsbourne and Moscovitch, 1990)
Your Bibliography: Reuter-Lorenz, P., Kinsbourne, M. and Moscovitch, M., 1990. Hemispheric control of spatial attention. Brain and Cognition, 12(2), pp.240-266.
In-text: (Salamé and Baddeley, 1989)
Your Bibliography: Salamé, P. and Baddeley, A., 1989. Effects of background music on phonological short-term memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 41(1), pp.107-122.
In-text: (Sampaio and Chokron, 1992)
Your Bibliography: Sampaio, E. and Chokron, S., 1992. Pseudoneglect and reversed pseudoneglect among left-handers and right-handers. Neuropsychologia, 30(9), pp.797-805.
In-text: (Sampaio and Philip, 1991)
Your Bibliography: Sampaio, E. and Philip, J., 1991. Sensory and motor aspects of pseudoneglect, hemifield, and hemispace in the tactile modality. Brain and Cognition, 16(1), pp.74-82.
In-text: (Swan, 2001)
Your Bibliography: Swan, L., 2001. Unilateral spatial neglect. Physical Therapy, 81(9), pp.1572-1580.
In-text: (Toba, Cavanagh and Bartolomeo, 2011)
Your Bibliography: Toba, M., Cavanagh, P. and Bartolomeo, P., 2011. Attention biases the perceived midpoint of horizontal lines. Neuropsychologia, 49(2), pp.238-246.
In-text: (Varnava and Halligan, 2007)
Your Bibliography: Varnava, A. and Halligan, P., 2007. Influence of Age and Sex on Line Bisection: A Study of Normal Performance with Implications for Visuospatial Neglect. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14(6), pp.571-585.
In-text: (Varnava, Dervinis and Chambers, 2013)
Your Bibliography: Varnava, A., Dervinis, M. and Chambers, C., 2013. The Predictive Nature of Pseudoneglect for Visual Neglect: Evidence from Parietal Theta Burst Stimulation. PLoS ONE, 8(6), p.e65851.
In-text: (Varnava, McCarthy and Beaumont, 2002)
Your Bibliography: Varnava, A., McCarthy, M. and Beaumont, J., 2002. Line bisection in normal adults: direction of attentional bias for near and far space. Neuropsychologia, 40(8), pp.1372-1378.
In-text: (Zorzi, Priftis and Umiltà, 2002)
Your Bibliography: Zorzi, M., Priftis, K. and Umiltà, C., 2002. Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417(6885), pp.138-139.
10,587 students joined last month!